Over the course of many years, without making any great fuss about it, the authorities in New York disabled most of the control buttons that once operated pedestrian-crossing lights in the city. Computerised timers, they had decided, almost always worked better. By 2004, fewer than 750 of 3,250 such buttons remained functional. The city government did not, however, take the disabled buttons away—beckoning countless fingers to futile pressing.
Initially, the buttons survived because of the cost of removing them. But it turned out that even inoperative buttons serve a purpose. Pedestrians who press a button are less likely to cross before the green man appears, says Tal Oron-Gilad of Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, in Israel. Having studied behaviour at crossings, she notes that people more readily obey a system which purports to heed their input.
Inoperative buttons produce placebo effects of this sort because people like an impression of control over systems they are using, says Eytan Adar, an expert on human-computer interaction at the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. Dr Adar notes that his students commonly design software with a clickable “save” button that has no role other than to reassure those users who are unaware that their keystrokes are saved automatically anyway. Think of it, he says, as a touch of benevolent deception to counter the inherent coldness of the machine world.
That is one view. But, at road crossings at least, placebo buttons may also have a darker side. Ralf Risser, head of FACTUM, a Viennese institute that studies psychological factors in traffic systems, reckons that pedestrians’ awareness of their existence, and consequent resentment at the deception, now outweighs the benefits. | Tokom niza godina, bez stvaranja mnogo buke oko toga, nadležni u Njujorku su onesposobili većinu dugmadi koja su nekada kontrolisala semafor za prelaz pešaka preko zebre u gradu. Kompjuterizovani semafori, kako su oni odlučili, skoro uvek rade bolje. Do 2004-te, manje nego 750 od 3,250 takvih dugmadi je ostalo funkcionalno. Gradske vlasti, međutim, nisu sklonile onesposobljenu dugmad -- navodeći bezbrojne prste na jalovo pritiskanje. Inicijalno, dugmad su ostala zbog cene njihovog uklanjanja. Ali ispostavilo se da čak i nefunkcionišuća dugmad imaju svoju ulogu. Manja je verovatnoća da će pešaci koji pritisnu dugme početi da prelaze ulicu pre nego što se pojavi zeleni čovek, kaže Tal Oron-Gilad sa Ben-Gurion universiteta u Negevu, Izrael. Studirajući ponašanja na prelazima, ona je zaključila da ljudi mnogo više poštuju sistem koji im daje utisak da ih sluša. Nefunkcionišuća dugmad proizvode ovakav placebo efekat zato što ljudi vole da imaju utisak kontrole nad sistemima koje koriste, kaže Ejtan Adar, specjalista za odnose izmedju ljudi i kompjutera sa Mičigenskog Univerziteta u An Arboru. Dr. Adar navodi da njegovi studenti često dizajniraju programe sa "sačuvaj" dugmetom na koje može da se klikne, a koje nema nikakvu drugu funkciju nego da stvori osećaj sigurosti kod korisnika koji ne znaju da su svi njihovi klikovi na tastaturi automatski sačuvani u svakom slučaju. Mislite o tome, kaže on, kao o maloj, benignoj laži koja služi kao protivteža urođenoj hladnoći sveta mašina. To je jedno mišljenje. Međutim, barem na prelazima, placebo dugmad mogu da imaju i mračniju stranu. Ralf Rajser, na čelu FACTUM-a, Bečkog instituta koji istražuje psihološke činioce u saobraćajnim sistemima, misli da znanje pešaka da ta dugmad postoje i njihovo nezadovoljstvo kao posledica te prevare, sada prevaguje u odnosu na njihovu korist. |