Over the course of many years, without making any great fuss about it, the authorities in New York disabled most of the control buttons that once operated pedestrian-crossing lights in the city. Computerised timers, they had decided, almost always worked better. By 2004, fewer than 750 of 3,250 such buttons remained functional. The city government did not, however, take the disabled buttons away—beckoning countless fingers to futile pressing.
Initially, the buttons survived because of the cost of removing them. But it turned out that even inoperative buttons serve a purpose. Pedestrians who press a button are less likely to cross before the green man appears, says Tal Oron-Gilad of Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, in Israel. Having studied behaviour at crossings, she notes that people more readily obey a system which purports to heed their input.
Inoperative buttons produce placebo effects of this sort because people like an impression of control over systems they are using, says Eytan Adar, an expert on human-computer interaction at the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. Dr Adar notes that his students commonly design software with a clickable “save” button that has no role other than to reassure those users who are unaware that their keystrokes are saved automatically anyway. Think of it, he says, as a touch of benevolent deception to counter the inherent coldness of the machine world.
That is one view. But, at road crossings at least, placebo buttons may also have a darker side. Ralf Risser, head of FACTUM, a Viennese institute that studies psychological factors in traffic systems, reckons that pedestrians’ awareness of their existence, and consequent resentment at the deception, now outweighs the benefits. | Tokom mnogo godina, bez dizanja ikakve velike galame oko toga, vlasti u Nju Jorku su onesposobile najveći broj kontrolnih dugmića koji su nekada upravljali svetlima na pešačkim prelazima u gradu. Kompjuterizovani tajmeri, vlasti su odlučile, skoro uvek su radili bolje. Od 2004. godine, manje od 750 od 3,250 tih dugmića ostalo je u funkciji. Gradska vlada nije, ipak, uklonila onesposobljene dugmiće — pozivajući nebrojene prste da ih uzaludno pritiskaju. U početku, dugmići su preživeli usled troška njihovog uklanjanja. Ali ispostavilo se da su čak i dugmići koji ne rade služili svrsi. Pešaci koji pritisnu dugme ređe su skloni da pređu pre nego što se zeleni čikica pojavi, kaže Tal Oron-Gilad s Univerziteta Ben-Gurion u Negevu, u Izraelu. Proučavajući ponašanje na prelazima, ona primećuje da ljudi radije poslušaju sistem koji se predstavlja kao da se obazire na njihov input. Dugmići koji ne rade proizvode placebo efekat ove vrste pošto se ljudima dopada utisak da kontrolišu sistem koji koriste, kaže Ejtan Adar, ekspert u interakciji čovek-kompjuter na Univerzitetu u Mičigenu, En Arbor. Dr Adar primećuje da njegovi studenti obično dizajniraju softvere sa dugmetom “sačuvaj” na koje može da se klikne, a koje nema drugu ulogu osim da umiri one korisnike koji nemaju svest o tome da su njihovi pritisci na taster svakako automatski sačuvani. Mislite o tome, kaže on, kao o maloj dobronamernoj prevari kojom odgovarate na hladnoću svojstvenu svetu mašina. To je jedno viđenje. Ali, na prelazima barem, placebo dugmad mogu da imaju i tamniju stranu. Ralf Riser, čelnik FACTUM-a, bečkog instituta koji proučava psihološke faktore u saobraćajnim sistemima, smatra da svest pešaka o njihovom postojanju, i posledična srdžba usled te prevare, sada nadilaze koristi. |